Lesson

Squirreling Around for Science: Observing Sciurid Rodents to Investigate Animal Behavior

Author(s): Patrice K. Connors*1, Johanna Varner1, Liesl P. Erb2, Laurie Dizney3, Hayley C. Lanier4, John D. Hanson5, Christopher J. Yahnke6, Jennifer M. Duggan7, Elizabeth A. Flaherty8

1. Colorado Mesa University 2. Warren Wilson College 3. University of Portland 4. University of Oklahoma 5. Institute for Biodiversity Research and Education 6. University of Wisconsin Stevens Point 7. California State University, Monterey Bay 8. Purdue University

Editor: Tammy Long

Published online:

Courses: EcologyEcology Introductory BiologyIntroductory Biology Science Process SkillsScience Process Skills

Keywords: foraging behavior behavioral ecology Sciurid Squirrel Squirrel-Net Trade-off Vigilance

3169 total view(s), 1094 download(s)

to access supporting documents

Abstract

Resource Image

Hands-on research experiences are important opportunities for students to learn about the nature of inquiry and gain confidence in solving problems. Here, we present an inquiry-based lesson plan that investigates the foraging behavior of sciurid rodents (squirrels) in local habitats. Squirrels are an ideal study system for student research projects because many species are diurnal, easy to watch, and inhabit a range of habitats including college campuses. In this activity, instructors identify appropriate field sites and focal species, while students generate questions and brainstorm predictions in small groups regarding factors that might influence behavioral trade-offs in sciurids. Students conduct observational surveys of local squirrels in pairs using a standardized protocol and upload their data to a national database as part of the multi-institutional Squirrel-Net (http://squirrel-net.org). Instructors access the nationwide dataset through the Squirrel-Net website and provide students with data for independent analysis. Students across the country observe and record a range of squirrel species, including behaviors and habitat characteristics. The national dataset can be used to answer student questions about why squirrels behave in the way they do and for students to learn about authentic analyses regarding behavior trade-offs. Additionally, the lesson is designed to be modified across a range of inquiry levels, from a single two-hour laboratory activity to a unit- or semester-long student-driven course-based research experience. Our activity highlights the value of using observational data to conduct research, makes use of the Squirrel-Net infrastructure for collaboration, and provides students equitable access to field-based projects with small mammals.

Primary image: Students observe squirrel behaviors on the campus of Colorado Mesa University.

Additional Squirrel-Net Articles:

Citation

Connors PK, Varner J, Erb LP, Dizney L, Lanier HC, Hanson JD, Yahnke CJ, Duggan JM, Flaherty EA. 2020. Squirreling around for science: Observing sciurid rodents to investigate animal behavior. CourseSource. https://doi.org/10.24918/cs.2020.7

Society Learning Goals

Ecology
Science Process Skills
  • Process of Science
    • Pose testable questions and hypotheses to address gaps in knowledge
    • Interpret, evaluate, and draw conclusions from data
    • Construct explanations and make evidence-based arguments about the natural world
  • Modeling/ Developing and Using Models
    • Recognize the important roles that scientific models, of many different types (conceptual, mathematical, physical, etc.), play in predicting and communicating biological phenomena
    • Make inferences and solve problems using models and simulations
    • Build and evaluate models of biological systems
  • Quantitative Reasoning/ Using Mathematics and Computational Thinking
    • Apply the tools of graphing, statistics, and data science to analyze biological data
  • Communication and Collaboration
    • Share ideas, data, and findings with others clearly and accurately

Lesson Learning Goals

Students will:
  • develop a basic understanding of the scientific process.
  • gain confidence in their ability to conduct scientific research.
  • understand that observing animal behavior is an important tool to investigate species interactions.
  • analyze data and learn about behavioral trade-offs in local wildlife.
  • value the diversity of ecological roles and adaptations found within the mammalian family Sciuridae (squirrels).
  • create a connection with nature by leaving the classroom to visit field sites and observe species in their habitats.

Lesson Learning Objectives

Students will:
  • conduct animal behavior surveys with peers.
  • input data into an online database for focal behavior observations.
  • develop and test hypotheses concerning trade-offs among squirrel behaviors.
  • analyze data from the national Squirrel-Net database for focal behavior observations.

Article Context

Course
Article Type
Course Level
Bloom's Cognitive Level
Vision and Change Core Competencies
Vision and Change Core Concepts
Class Type
Class Size
Audience
Lesson Length
Pedagogical Approaches
Principles of How People Learn
Assessment Type

INTRODUCTION

What, how, and why an animal eats are some of the most fundamental questions in animal behavior. Coming from the Greek word trophē, meaning nourishment, the trophic biology of an animal provides valuable information about its diet, as well as its role in cycling nutrients and transferring energy through ecosystems. Such information is crucial for management and conservation of endangered or threatened species (1). Multiple theories in biology explain the dietary decision-making of animals (2). For instance, optimal foraging theory suggests that animals maximize their caloric intake over time (3). Animals balance many factors when foraging, such as time spent actively searching for food and total calories gained per meal. This balance of multiple factors is one of the many trade-offs studied in behavioral ecology. Trade-offs in biology imply that time or energy spent on one task, such as foraging, is time or energy that is not spent on another task, such as avoiding predators or finding a mate.

Studies of animal foraging behavior provide an opportunity to engage students in authentic, classroom-based research projects (4,5). Lesson plans that use camera traps, for example, offer a great opportunity for students to conduct data analyses, but lack the direct connection with wildlife (6). In contrast, in situ observations are the oldest and most direct way to record animal feeding activities. Observational surveys have answered questions regarding foraging decisions and trade-offs in species ranging from songbirds and birds of prey (7,8) to hippopotamuses and chipmunks (9,10). Moreover, observing live animals can foster personal connections with science and nature, particularly for students from urban settings who may be less familiar with wildlife (11). Behavioral surveys therefore provide unique opportunities to engage students in science (12).

Sciurid rodents (squirrels) are great candidates for student research activities in behavioral ecology for several reasons. Sciurids are charismatic and diurnal, and over 60 species are found across North America in habitats from natural areas to urban settings like college campuses (13–15). Like many prey species, squirrels display distinct foraging and vigilance behaviors, and must balance the time they spend eating versus watching for predators (16,17). Therefore, students can conduct behavioral surveys of local squirrel species and test predictions regarding trade-offs between foraging and other behaviors. Factors that can influence these behaviors include the presence of other species, habitat type, the degree of urbanization (18), social patterns of the focal species (19), and abiotic factors like temperature and precipitation (20). For instance, habitat structure could affect vulnerability and exposure to predation (e.g., a reduced understory could expose squirrels to aerial predators), which could in turn increase vigilance behavior in squirrels (21,22).

Here, we present a lesson plan that uses behavioral surveys to provide undergraduate students authentic research experiences in the classroom. To prepare for the activity, instructors identify local species and field sites, and students generate hypotheses and predictions concerning trade-offs between foraging and other behaviors in squirrels. During the lesson, students investigate these trade-offs by conducting behavioral surveys of local squirrels using a standardized protocol, and then input their data into a national database. Instructors can access the national dataset and provide students with a dataset that was compiled by students across the country. Students create graphs to test their hypotheses, ultimately leading to new knowledge and insights regarding animal behavior. Alternatively, instructors could extend the activity by teaching data analyses and interpretation in a successive lab period.

The present lesson has been tested with over 800 students across multiple institutions and in many different course types. Consequently, we have standardized the collection of behavior data while allowing flexibility in the level of inquiry. We believe these attributes strengthen this lesson plan and make it a valuable, highly adaptable tool to provide research experiences to students in the fields of ecology, animal behavior and mammalogy.

Rationale and Origin of Lesson

Squirrel-Net is a group of teacher-scholars that share a common goal of promoting authentic course-based research experiences (CUREs) for undergraduate students. We are mammalogists that hold research and teaching positions at higher educational institutions across the United States. Our goals are to create inquiry-based lesson plans that take students out of the classroom and engage them in research on locally relevant and widely distributed mammals, while also collecting data with standardized protocols to test a wide array of ecological questions across multiple spatial and temporal scales.

This lesson is part of the Squirrel-Net module series (http://squirrel-net.org). All of the Squirrel-Net modules are designed for adaptation to diverse educational contexts, from a single two-hour laboratory period (basic skills acquisition with structured inquiry) to a unit- or semester-long student-driven research project (open inquiry CURE). In each module, students submit data to a national dataset that aggregates observations from multiple institutions. Instructors can access the freely available national database, thereby allowing students to explore and analyze focal questions of the module across a broader variety of habitats and species than would be possible at a single institution. Finally, the four Squirrel-Net modules published in this set (23–25) are designed to be scaffolded into multiple levels in a curriculum, allowing students to return to a similar taxon and themes as well as uniting inquiry across different courses (26).

Intended Audience

Squirreling Around for Science is intended for undergraduate level courses in the sciences at any type of institution. From 2017-2019, this lesson was implemented at nine colleges and universities in the United States, which include small private primarily undergraduate institutions, mid-sized public institutions serving local, under-represented and non-traditional populations, and large public research institutions. This lesson is highly adaptable and has been taught in numerous course types, ranging from non-biology majors to introductory major courses, to upper division electives, with class sizes from 15 to 80 students.

Required Learning Time

One laboratory period (1 hour 50 minutes), at a minimum.

Prerequisite Student Knowledge

Students should be acquainted with the ecological concepts of species interactions and activity budgets. Specifically, students should be familiar with predator-prey relationships and the adaptations associated with each species (i.e., faster running or ambush speeds of predators, camouflage or physical defenses of prey). Trade-offs occur in activity budgets because there is only a set amount of time or energy available for all of an animal's activities. Animals are constantly making decisions about the amount of time or energy to spend on one activity such as foraging, thereby limiting the amount of time or energy available for another activity such as watching for predators. We also suggest that students be familiar with science process skills, including asking a question, developing a testable hypothesis and prediction based on prior knowledge, and using a standardized protocol to collect data. Additionally, students should have experience with reading and making graphs, either by hand and with computer software such as Microsoft Excel or R. Alternatively, this lesson is great for teaching about data analysis and graphing, but would necessitate at least one additional laboratory period.

Prerequisite Teacher Knowledge

Instructors need to identify a location to observe local squirrel species (Supporting File S1: Squirreling Around for Science – Resources for identifying focal species and field sites). We also suggest that instructors practice observing squirrels and filling out the squirrel behavior datasheet (Supporting File S2: Squirreling Around for Science – Squirrel behavior observation datasheet) before the lesson. It would be beneficial for instructors to have a basic knowledge of animal behavior (e.g., see resources in (27–32).

SCIENTIFIC TEACHING THEMES

Active Learning

During the lesson, we conduct small group and classroom-wide discussions for students to make predictions about foraging behavior. Discussions are facilitated with think-pair-share. Students collaborate to record behavior of local squirrels outside the classroom and analyze data collected across the country. Students also draw conclusions from those analyses in small groups and in classroom-wide discussions.

Assessment

Basic lesson assessment for this activity includes the creation of a graph and a one-page written summary. Rubrics are provided to students during the lesson, which include evidence of student participation in group discussions and data collection. We provide assessment examples (Supporting File S3: Squirreling Around for Science – Supplies for introductory majors or lower division majors course, Supporting File S4: Squirreling Around for Science – Supplies for non-majors course and Supporting File S5: Squirreling Around for Science – Supplies for upper division course) and modifications to the assessments (see Teaching Discussion and Table 2). 

Inclusive Teaching

Squirrel-Net modules are designed to provide all students in a class with the opportunity to engage in authentic research experiences. Participation in traditional research experiences can be limited to a select group of students, either because few opportunities exist or because students have other commitments outside of their education (33). By bringing research into the classroom, it can more seamlessly become part of the educational experience for all students. Furthermore, participating in a CURE has significant impacts on students’ sense of self-efficacy as a scientist and may promote retention in science, particularly for students from under-represented groups (34). One strength of the Squirrel-Net modules is the use of a national network, which will further help students feel they are making important contributions and belong to a broader scientific community beyond their classroom or institution (35).

This lesson supports multiple modes of learning. Students have the opportunity to work independently and/or collaboratively as they complete each step of the lesson. For instance, students engage in active inquiry and work together to collect their own behavior data. This activity is unique because it physically takes students outside the classroom and connects students with animals in their local environment. Students also work in small groups to generate their own hypotheses and predictions about foraging behavior trade-offs. Lastly, this lesson is not a “cookbook” lab activity with known outcomes, but instead guides students through the scientific process, encouraging and celebrating novel discoveries regarding animal behavior.

LESSON PLAN

Pre-class preparation

Before conducting the lesson, instructors should determine their focal squirrel species and field site location(s). There are multiple, open-access resources available online to help instructors correctly identify their local sciurid species (Supporting File S1: Squirreling Around for Science – Resources for identifying focal species and field sites). We also provide important tips when identifying a species and study site close to the classroom. For example, when visiting a potential site, instructors should record the time it takes to travel from the classroom to the field site and back, to ensure that students can successfully conduct their observational surveys within the class timeframe. Many college campuses provide suitable habitat for squirrels, and some institutions may have natural areas on their campus specifically for ecological research and hands-on education. Local parks and public lands are other options for finding sciurids.

If instructors are unfamiliar with collecting focal animal observations using an ethogram, we suggest they practice recording data prior to the lesson, using the datasheet (Supporting File S2: Squirreling Around for Science – Squirrel behavior observation datasheet), student instructions (Supporting File S6: Squirreling Around for Science – Squirrel behavior student directions), and squirrel behavior ethogram (Supporting File S7: Squirreling Around for Science – Squirrel behavior ethogram table). Instructors can practice filling out the datasheet by watching a freely-available video clip of uninterrupted squirrel behavior, such as those available on our website (http://squirrel-net.org). The completed sample datasheet can serve as an example for students during the lesson.

Prior to the lesson, instructors should familiarize themselves with the national database, which is available by request through our website. This dataset may be manipulated to produce a graph for assessment and/or potentially edit or cleaned for student analyses (see Teaching Discussion for modifications). An example datasheet with both original data from the database and a cleaned data set specific to answering student questions is provided (Supporting File S8: Squirreling Around for Science – Example of national database). Finally Squirrel-Net has also developed several videos introducing the network and the protocols for this lesson; these videos are available via our website (http://www.squirrel-net.org).

Handouts and materials to be prepared before the lesson include the datasheet (Supporting File S2: Squirreling Around for Science – Squirrel behavior observation datasheet), student instructions (Supporting File S6: Squirreling Around for Science – Squirrel behavior student directions), and ethogram (Supporting File S7: Squirreling Around for Science – Squirrel behavior ethogram table). Optional documents include any additional worksheets pertaining to specific focal species and rubrics for assessments (e.g., Supporting Files S3: Squirreling Around for Science – Supplies for introductory majors or lower division majors course, S4: Squirreling Around for Science – Supplies for non-majors course and S5: Squirreling Around for Science – Supplies for upper division course). Required materials for surveys include a writing surface (e.g., clipboard), a GPS device (or a smartphone), and a timepiece (e.g., a watch, phone timer, or stopwatch). Most students can provide these items themselves, especially since they will be collecting data in groups. For instance, modern smartphones include a stopwatch and mapping capabilities that provide GPS coordinates in decimal degrees (either by dropping a pin at the current location in Google Maps or by using a free GPS app, such as GAIA GPS or HandyGPS). Depending on focal species and its distance from observers, students should also bring binoculars.

Progressing Through the Lesson

Interactive lecture (~15 minutes).

We begin the lesson with a short, interactive lecture about squirrels and foraging behaviors (Supporting File S9: Squirreling Around for Science – Lecture slides with background information). Instructors can focus on background material appropriate to the course. For example, in a non-majors biology course that broadly covers animal biology, we focus on squirrel biology and emphasize locally occurring species. In upper division courses, we focus on broader theories of foraging behavior (e.g., optimal foraging theory). Some instructors use short video clips to introduce topics to students. We also introduce Squirrel-Net, highlighting the fact that students like themselves across the country are collecting data and contributing to the dataset. We end the lecture by describing the behavior observation surveys. Specifically, we hand out instructions (Supporting File S6: Squirreling Around for Science – Squirrel behavior student directions), datasheets (Supporting File S2: Squirreling Around for Science – Squirrel behavior observation datasheet) and ethograms (Supporting File S7: Squirreling Around for Science – Squirrel behavior ethogram table), explain specific squirrel behaviors outlined in the ethogram (Supporting File S9: Squirreling Around for Science – Lecture slides with background information, slide 9), and walk through the instructions for completing the datasheet.

Practice data collection (<10 minutes).

Once students have all the handouts, instructors usually lead a practice recording observational data. We play a practice video (available on our website: http://squirrel-net.org) and prompt students to categorize the squirrel's behavior based on the ethogram at 20-second intervals. We allow students to record for a full five minutes to simulate how long the procedure should last. After students record their data, we review the behaviors as a whole class and clarify any questions or confusion about the protocol.

Generate questions and predictions (15-20 minutes).

In pairs or small groups, students brainstorm factors that influence the foraging behaviors of squirrels. They then work together to determine possible questions and hypotheses that could be tested with the data. The data include squirrel behavior, date, time, weather, habitat and GPS location. Thus, students are able to learn and ask questions about the effects of urbanization, abiotic factors, and presence of other animals on activity budgets. Possible questions include: "Will squirrels spend more time being vigilant on college campuses compared to natural environments?", "Do different species of squirrels show similar behaviors on college campuses across the United States?", or "What is the effect of season on the behavior of squirrels found on college campuses?" We have students share their questions with the whole class and discuss how the data can be used to answer each question. We allow each pair or group of students to test a different question, but some instructors may choose to have only one question that the whole class will test (see Teaching Discussion for modifications). Students also brainstorm predictions in small groups or in pairs. In classes with a quantitative/statistical component, we challenge students to sketch a figure of their expected results; this provides practice in graph-making skills and prepares students to interpret data later. Students often create bar graphs to clearly compare average behavioral trends in categories such as location, habitat type, behavior type or species.

Conduct surveys (~45 minutes).

Prior to conducting outside surveys, we recommend reviewing the instructions, then leading students to a predetermined field site. The protocol requires students to work in pairs: one student watches the focal animal and classifies its behavior, while a second student calls out 20-second intervals and records observations on the datasheet. Groups of three students are possible for larger classes or those with odd numbers; in this case, there will be one observer, one data recorder, and one timekeeper. However, student groups of four or more are not recommended because there are not enough roles to keep all students engaged in the activity. We ask students to rotate roles such that every student has an opportunity to be the recorder and the observer. Once at the field site, students should find a squirrel before filling out the datasheet. Instructors should ensure that students are not recording the same animal at the same time. One pair of students can record the same squirrel at different times if the animal remains in the study site for the entire length of the observational time (i.e., more than 10 minutes).

Database input (<10 minutes per student).

Upon returning to the classroom, we provide time for students to enter their data into the national database. Instructors can request access to the national dataset and the data entry links via our website (http://www.squirrel-net.org). We suggest that students complete this task in class using their own mobile devices in case questions arise. Instructors can also collect hard copy datasheets from students after data entry for back-up, quality control, and/or attendance.

Lesson wrap-up (<10 minutes).

We conclude the lesson by summarizing the activity, introducing the assignment, and providing a rubric (examples in Supporting Files S3: Squirreling Around for Science – Supplies for introductory majors or lower division majors course, S4: Squirreling Around for Science – Supplies for non-majors course and S5: Squirreling Around for Science – Supplies for upper division course). For an introductory or lower-division course, we assign a short, one-page written report that asks students to state the question, explain a hypothesis, and use the class' data or the national database to make a graph and interpret data to determine if the results support the hypothesis. Rubrics can also require summary statistics, such as means and standard deviations.

Outside of Class Activities

Instructors can request access to the national database for this module through our website (http://www.squirrel-net.org). Data are available in Excel spreadsheets and can easily be manipulated in Excel (i.e., for data cleaning or streamlining). We suggest instructors download data soon after their class submits their data to provide time for cleaning, or the removal of certain fields based on student questions and data quality (example sheets in Supporting File S8: Squirreling Around for Science – Example of national database). For example, instructors may restrict the dataset to a single squirrel species or habitat, or remove extraneous variables before sharing the dataset with students. In lower-division courses, we removed incomplete observations and only provided students with the data necessary to test their hypotheses, thereby reducing confusion about data analysis. Instructors are encouraged to tailor data cleaning to the appropriate inquiry level for their course.

TEACHING DISCUSSION

Collectively, we have successfully implemented this lesson over 20 times during the 2017-2019 academic years in 13 courses at nine institutions across the United States. The courses ranged from non-majors courses to lower and upper division major courses, and we estimate that over 800 students have been engaged in this lesson. As a result, we now have a large national database (available by request via our website), and such datasets tend to be more forgiving of occasional erroneous observations. All instructors, including those not specifically trained in behavioral ecology or mammalogy, provided positive feedback about the procedure and ease of data entry. Student evaluations showed that students enjoyed the outdoors and hands-on aspect of the lesson, contributing to a nationwide study, and asking their own research questions. Informal evaluations from students and instructors alike suggested that some students gained confidence in their ability to conduct scientific research and became more interested in careers in science after participation in the lesson. We believe this evidence suggests that our lesson plan enhances students' research skills and confidence in research.

Extensions and Modifications

The current lesson is written at the controlled level of inquiry (Table 2); however, it can be easily modified to provide a more structured or more student-driven inquiry, depending on the instructor's goals and course context. For example, instructors of non-biology majors courses can use this lesson at the structured inquiry level (Table 2). Here, instructors provide a single research question and students generate hypotheses as a whole class. Instructors take students to a single field site and observe a single focal species during a single 2-hour class period. After data collection, the instructor provides a graph for interpretation and discussion as a whole class, instead of a dataset for student analyses. Other modifications are also possible at this level of inquiry. For instance, the lesson was implemented during a 50-minute lecture period for 30 non-major students. We presented background information and practiced recording behaviors with the class, but students collected data outside of class time (e.g., over the weekend). In a second lecture period, students entered data they collected in a computer lab, generated potential questions they could ask from the data, and then predicted possible responses with a structured worksheet (Supporting File S4: Squirreling Around for Science – Supplies for non-majors course).

To adapt the current lesson plan to meet higher levels of inquiry (Table 2), instructors can provide students with a choice of field sites and/or focal species or encourage students to select their own observation sites outside of class. Instructors can link hypothesis development to background information such as ecological concepts, like trophic levels, or animal behavior theories, like optimal foraging theory. Example research questions at this level of inquiry could include "How does urbanization of habitat affect the perception of risk for sciurids?", "Do changes in perception of risk promote acclimation to urban habitats?", or "Do sciurids adapt to changes in climate by altering phenology of caching?" We often suggest that students in upper division classes collect observational data multiple times. Therefore, this lesson plan requires more than one laboratory period that is at least two hours long or data collection outside of class time. During the additional data collection sessions, instructors can provide feedback to student groups about their specific question and prediction. Students then analyze a cleaned or raw dataset provided by the instructor and create a one-page summary with graphs and simple statistics or a larger lab report with an introduction and conclusion (S5: Supplies for upper division course). Alternatively, instructors can employ an “un-essay” final project format, whereby students are allowed to submit a data-driven artistic expression of their questions and findings (e.g., a painting, rap song, sculpture, or cartoon (36,37)). 

A unique strength of the Squirrel-Net modules is that they can be adapted at multiple levels, allowing students to revisit the same research topic at different levels in their education. Specifically, Colorado Mesa University students participated in Squirreling Around for Science in both lower-division required courses at a structured inquiry level and again in upper-division elective courses at an open inquiry level. During the second implementation of the activity, we focused on higher-level research skills, such as more-complex statistical analyses and dealing with messy and/or incomplete data. Far from feeling like the lesson was redundant or duplicative, many students appreciated the opportunity to revisit the activity. Informal conversations with students suggest that they gained an appreciation for the research process by comparing their experience in open inquiry to their previous analysis with a cleaned dataset. Repeated exposure to the same topic is known to promote deeper engagement and long-term gains in skills and content knowledge (38). Finally, if instructors plan to employ multiple Squirrel-Net modules (e.g., (23–25)), we recommend Squirreling Around for Science as an entry-level lesson to the network and to the concepts of animal behavior.

Squirreling Around for Science can also be extended into a unit- or semester-long CURE. This free inquiry lesson plan is aimed at senior-level students and can be used as a capstone activity for science majors. Here, instructors create a longer timeline that allows students to perform research of primary literature in order to generate questions and hypotheses about sciurid foraging behavior across a range of species and habitat types. Students collect observational data multiple times and are provided a raw dataset that includes multiple locations and species across time. Students conduct statistical analyses ranging from t-tests to ANOVAs, and then write a publication-style paper with references or create a conference-style presentation. Opportunities exist within this format to cover more specific topics related to the course, to incorporate other resources on campus such as primary literature research, and to test open-ended and novel questions with the national dataset. Furthermore, student pre- and post-assessment surveys regarding the effectiveness of the CURE can be requested via the Squirrel-Net homepage (http://squirrel-net.org), which are in compliance with the Institutional Review Board for the Protection of Human Subjects in Research (IRB).

Finally, instructors could adapt the current lesson to investigate the behavioral ecology of animals beyond sciurid rodents. We focus on squirrels because of their charismatic behavior, diurnal activity, and ubiquity across habitats, but the observational protocol described here can be easily modified to record behaviors from other mammalian species. Ungulates (e.g., deer) and lagomorphs (e.g., rabbits) are prey species that must also balance foraging with predator surveillance and are common across a range of habitat types. Alternatively, the lesson plan can be adapted to observe bird behaviors. Feeders in urban and suburban habitats offer an opportunity to test predictions about trade-offs associated with foraging behaviors, species interactions (e.g., competition with conspecifics, predator vs. prey), and the influence of migration. In summary, Squirreling Around for Science is designed to be easily modified to meet instructor needs based on time, content, and assessment goals while providing students with authentic research experiences studying local mammals outdoors.

SUPPORTING MATERIALS

  • S1. Squirreling Around for Science – Resources for identifying focal species and field sites.
  • S2. Squirreling Around for Science – Squirrel behavior observation datasheet.
  • S3 Squirreling Around for Science – Supplies for introductory majors or lower division majors course.
  • S4. Squirreling Around for Science – Supplies for non-majors course.
  • S5. Squirreling Around for Science – Supplies for upper division course.
  • S6. Squirreling Around for Science – Squirrel behavior student directions.
  • S7. Squirreling Around for Science – Squirrel behavior ethogram table.
  • S8. Squirreling Around for Science – Example of national database.
  • S9. Squirreling Around for Science – Lecture slides with background information.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

PKC, JV and JMD were supported by the 2019 CourseSource Writing Studio to develop this article. PKC and JV were supported by the Colorado Mesa University Faculty Professional Development Fund. JMD received support from California State University, Monterey Bay through a Faculty Support Grant and a CURE Fellowship. Support for EAF was provided by the USDA National Institute of Food and Agriculture, Hatch Project 1019737. CJY was supported by a UW System Teaching Fellows grant. This material is based upon work supported by the National Science Foundation under a collaborative grant (Nos. 2013483, 2013281, 2013308, and 2013320). We would also like to thank all of the students who provided helpful feedback on these modules as we piloted them in our courses.

References

  1. Doniol-Valcroze T, Lesage V, Giard J, Michaud R. 2011. Optimal foraging theory predicts diving and feeding strategies of the largest marine predator. Behav Ecol 22:880-888.
  2. Senft RL, Coughenour MB, Bailey DW, Rittenhouse LR, Sala OE, Swift DM. 1987. Large Herbivore Foraging and Ecological Hierarchies. BioScience 37:789-799.
  3. Schoener TW. 1987. A Brief History of Optimal Foraging Ecology, p. 5-67. In Kamil, AC, Krebs, JR, Pulliam, HR (eds.), Foraging Behavior. Springer US, Boston, MA.
  4. Martin P, Bateson P. 2007. Measuring Behaviour: An Introductory Guide. Cambridge University Press.
  5. McCleery RA, Lopez RR, Harveson LA, Silvy NJ, Slack RD. 2005. Integrating on-campus wildlife research projects into the wildlife curriculum. Wildl Soc Bull 33:802-809.
  6. Wu XB, Knight SL, Schielack JF, Robledo DC, Jaime XA, Peterson CA, Griffing LR. 2016. Authentic Ecological Inquiries Using BearCam Archives. CourseSource. https://doi.org/10.24918/cs.2016.22
  7. Kamil AC, Roitblat HL. 1985. The Ecology of Foraging Behavior: Implications for Animal Learning and Memory. Annu Rev Psychol 36:141-169.
  8. Rudolph SG. 1982. Foraging Strategies of American Kestrels During Breeding. Ecology 63:1268-1276.
  9. Lewison RL, Carter J. 2004. Exploring behavior of an unusual megaherbivore: a spatially explicit foraging model of the hippopotamus. Ecol Model 171:127-138.
  10. Kramer DL, Nowell W. 1980. Central place foraging in the Eastern chipmunk, Tamias striatus. Anim Behav 28:772-778.
  11. Myers Jr O, Saunders C, Bexell S. 2009. Fostering empathy with wildlife: Factors affecting free-choice learning for conservation concern and behavior. Free-Choice Learn Environ 39-55.
  12. Grove P. 2011. Why Should Our Students Study Animal Behavior? Am Biol Teach 73:206.
  13. Eder T. 2009. Squirrels of North America. Lone Pine, Edmonton ; Auburn, Wash.
  14. Booth WC, Colomb GG, Williams JM. 2008. Chapter 4: From Questions to a ProblemThe Craft of Research, Third Edition. University Of Chicago Press, Chicago.
  15. Peplinski J, Brown JS. Distribution and diversity of squirrels on university and college campuses of the United States and Canada. J Mammal. https://doi.org/10.1093/jmammal/gyaa033
  16. Makowska IJ, Kramer DL. 2007. Vigilance during food handling in grey squirrels, Sciurus carolinensis. Animal Behaviour 74:153–158.
  17. Hannon MJ, Jenkins SH, Crabtree RL, Swanson AK. 2006. Visibility and Vigilance: Behavior and Population Ecology of Uinta Ground Squirrels (Spermophilus armatus) in Different Habitats. J Mammal 87:287–295.
  18. Chapman T, Rymer T, Pillay N. 2012. Behavioural correlates of urbanisation in the Cape ground squirrel Xerus inauris. Naturwissenschaften 99:893–902.
  19. Olson RS, Haley PB, Dyer FC, Adami C. 2015. Exploring the evolution of a trade-off between vigilance and foraging in group-living organisms. R Soc Open Sci 2:150135.
  20. Bachman GC. 1993. The effect of body condition on the trade-off between vigilance and foraging in Belding’s ground squirrels. Animal Behaviour 46:233–244.
  21. Sharpe PB, Van Horne B. 1998. Influence of Habitat on Behavior of Townsend’s Ground Squirrels (Spermophilus townsendii). J Mammal 79:906–918.
  22. Brownell SE, Freeman S, Wenderoth MP, Crowe AJ. 2014. BioCore Guide: A Tool for Interpreting the Core Concepts of Vision and Change for Biology Majors. LSE 13:200–211.
  23. Varner J, Lanier HC, Duggan JM, Dizney L, Flaherty EA, Connors PK, Erb LP, Yahnke CJ, Hanson JD. 2020. How many squirrels are in the shrubs? A lesson plan for comparing methods for population estimation. CourseSource. https://doi.org/10.24918/cs.2020.6
  24. Yahnke CJ, Dizney L, Varner J, Duggan JM, Erb LP, Lanier HC, Flaherty EA, Connors PK, Hanson JD. 2020. Sorry to eat and run: A lesson plan for testing trade-off in squirrel behavior using Giving Up Densities (GUDs). CourseSource. https://doi.org/10.24918/cs.2020.30
  25. Duggan JM, Varner J, Lanier HC, Flaherty EA, Dizney L, Yahnke CJ, Connors PK, Erb LP, Hanson JD. 2020. Squirrels in space: Using radio telemetry to explore the space use and movement of sciurid rodents. CourseSource. https://doi.org/10.24918/cs.2020.25
  26. Dizney L, Connors PK, Varner J, Duggan JM, Lanier HC, Erb LP, Flaherty EA, Yahnke CJ, Hanson JD. 2020. An introduction to the Squirrel-Net teaching modules. CourseSource. https://doi.org/10.24918/cs.2020.26
  27. Ridley M. 1995. Animal Behavior: An Introduction to Behavioral Mechanisms, Development, and Ecology2 edition. Wiley-Blackwell, Boston.
  28. Young J. 2012. Animal Behavior, p. 462–479. In Silvy, NJ (ed.), The Wildlife Techniques Manual, 7th ed. Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, MD.
  29. Tenney S. Animal Behavior | Learn Science at Scitable.
  30. Garland T. 2014. Trade-offs. Current Biology 24:R60–R61.
  31. Derr. Evolutionary Trade-offs | BEACON.
  32. Optimal Foraging - an overview | ScienceDirect Topics.
  33. Kim YK, Sax LJ. 2009. Student–Faculty Interaction in Research Universities: Differences by Student Gender, Race, Social Class, and First-Generation Status. Res High Educ 50:437–459.
  34. Rainey K, Dancy M, Mickelson R, Stearns E, Moller S. 2018. Race and gender differences in how sense of belonging influences decisions to major in STEM. International Journal of STEM Education 5:10.
  35. Dewsbury B, Brame C. 2019. Inclusive Teaching. CBE—Life Sciences Education 18.
  36. Sullivan P. 2015. The UnEssay: Making Room for Creativity in the Composition Classroom. College Composition and Communication 67:6–34.
  37. Jakopak RP, Monteith KL, Merkle BG. 2019. Writing Science: Improving Understanding and Communication Skills with the “Unessay.” The Bulletin of the Ecological Society of America 100:e01610.
  38. Felder R, Brent R. 2016. Teaching and Learning STEM: A Practical Guide. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, CA.

Article Files

to access supporting documents

  • pdf Squirreling Around for Science: Observing Sciurid Rodents to Investigate Animal Behavior(PDF | 205 KB)
  • docx S1. Squirreling Around for Science Resources for identifying focal species and field sites.docx(DOCX | 9 KB)
  • pdf S2. Squirreling Around for Science Squirrel behavior observation datasheet.pdf(PDF | 27 KB)
  • docx S3. Squirreling Around for Science Supplies for introductory majors or lower division majors course.docx(DOCX | 18 KB)
  • docx S4. Squirreling Around for Science Supplies for non-majors course.docx(DOCX | 25 KB)
  • docx S5. Squirreling Around for Science Supplies for upper division course.docx(DOCX | 21 KB)
  • docx S6. Squirreling Around for Science Squirrel behavior student directions.docx(DOCX | 18 KB)
  • docx S7. Squirreling Around for Science Squirrel behavior ethogram table.docx(DOCX | 3 MB)
  • xlsx S8. Squirreling Around for Science Example of national database.xlsx(XLSX | 94 KB)
  • pptx S9. Squirreling Around for Science Lecture slides with background information.pptx(PPTX | 8 MB)
  • License terms

Authors

Author(s): Patrice K. Connors*1, Johanna Varner1, Liesl P. Erb2, Laurie Dizney3, Hayley C. Lanier4, John D. Hanson5, Christopher J. Yahnke6, Jennifer M. Duggan7, Elizabeth A. Flaherty8

1. Colorado Mesa University 2. Warren Wilson College 3. University of Portland 4. University of Oklahoma 5. Institute for Biodiversity Research and Education 6. University of Wisconsin Stevens Point 7. California State University, Monterey Bay 8. Purdue University

About the Authors

*Correspondence to:  1100 North Ave, Grand Junction CO 81501. Email: pconnors@coloradomesa.edu.

Competing Interests

PKC, JV and JMD were supported by the 2019 CourseSource Writing Studio. PKC and JV were supported by the Colorado Mesa University Faculty Professional Development Fund. JMD received support from California State University, Monterey Bay through a Faculty Support Grant and a CURE Fellowship. Support for EAF was provided by the USDA National Institute of Food and Agriculture, Hatch Project 1019737. CJY was supported by a UW System Teaching Fellows grant. This material is based upon work supported by the National Science Foundation under a collaborative grant (Nos. 2013483, 2013281, 2013308, and 2013320). None of the authors has a financial, personal, or professional conflict of interest related to this work.

Comments

Comments

There are no comments on this resource.